Why would so many US working and middle class citizens
vote against their own class interests in the recent presidential
election?
And what can really be said about the “morality” of
those who oppose stem cell research, effective social programs
for the poor and disadvantaged, adequate funding of schools, abortion
and the right of women to control their bodies, national health
care insurance, and affirmative action programs but at the same
time support capital punishment, economic inequality with the most
extreme income gap between “haves” and “have-nots” of
any industrial nation, racism, both structural and otherwise, and
the criminal invasion of Iraq with the slaughter of at least 100,000
children, women, and men all for the sake of Iraqi oil and imperialist
dreams of conquest and domination, and white supremacist visions
of world hegemony?
Given such glaring contradictions, one wonders if many US citizens
are able to discern the difference between the wheat and the chaff
and what is the quality of their reality contact.
Or is there another dynamic lurking just beneath the surface: white control,
white exclusivity, and in a word, white supremacy under
the guise of so-called “Christian” values which are
often egregious violations of many of the basic tenets of authentic
Christianity including the Golden Rule, “Do unto others as
you would have them do unto you,” a rule predicated on liberty,
justice and equality, as well as other integrative aspects of Christianity
like “Love thy neighbor as thy self” which speaks of
socialism rather than unjust, misanthropic, dog-eat-dog capitalism
and class conflict. As Jesus said, “What thou doeth to the
least of these, thou doeth to me.” This contrast is
clear if you compare the teachings of the late Rev. Dr. Martin
Luther King, Jr. with those of Rev. Jerry Falwell.
Do you really think that the God of Christianity as revealed in
the Bible is unconcerned about the environment, or war and peace,
and injustice and poverty? Does God want 45 million Americans
to be without healthcare and does God support the racist exclusion
of people of color and other out groups?
So we see that the deeply ubiquitous ideology of Euro-American
supremacy trumps class interests and, in this instance, leads to
a tragic, dangerous, and irrational choice, the election of an
overtly racist, war criminal hell bent on destroying the last tattered
remnants of the social safety net and seems intent on attempting
to remake the world in accord with his own destructive, anti-democratic,
white supremacist vision: George Walker Bush.
Bush’s Iraqi imbroglio is simply a stale rehash of the tired,
old “White Man’s Burden” routine. When
he announces in his messianic, megalomaniac zeal he will bring
freedom and democracy to Iraq, he denies Iraqi cultural history,
Iraqi legitimacy, and Iraqi autonomy, and the value and worth of
the Iraqi people themselves. In the end, according to the
Bush plan, “Big Boss Man” will be in control,
in charge of the destiny of the Iraqi people, and more
to the real motive, in charge of their oil.
If one were to substitute the word, “Iraqis,” for
the word, “slaves,” a comment by a Euro-American holder
of enslaved Africans, “We turn our slaves into whatever we
want them to be” precisely mirrors Bush’s simple minded,
tragically ineffective Iraqi strategy which seems doomed to failure
but is likely to produce terrible consequences, to unleash the
proverbial whirlwind.
But again, from the Bush perspective, “Big Boss Man” rules
and Euro-American superiority and Bush et al.’s intrinsic
right to world hegemony are tacitly assumed.
Unfortunately, all too many Americans seem to agree, especially
Bush supporters.
Pertinent in this context is a recent survey by the Program on
International Policy Attitudes (PIPA),
School of Public Affairs, University of Maryland, “The Separate
Realities of Bush and Kerry Supporters,” that yielded surreal,
delusion-like results. The survey revealed that 72% of those who
support Bush still believe Iraq had weapons of mass destruction
or a major program for developing them while Kerry supporters had
opposite beliefs on all these points.
In addition, 75% of Bush supporters continue to believe that Iraq
was providing substantial support to al Qaeda and 63% believe solid,
irrefutable evidence of this was found. Sixty percent of
Bush supporters assume that this is also the conclusion of most
experts, and 55% assume, incorrectly, that this was the conclusion
of the 9/11 Commission. And again, large majorities of Kerry
supporters hold exactly opposite, more veridical views.
Steven Kull, director of PIPA, commented: “One of
the reasons that Bush supporters have these beliefs is that they
perceive the Bush administration confirming them. Interestingly,
this is one point on which Bush and Kerry supporters agree.” Eighty-two
percent of Bush supporters perceive the Bush administration as
asserting that Iraq had WMD (63%) or that Iraq had a major WMD
program (19%). Similarly, 75% say that the Bush administration
has indicated that Iraq was providing substantial support to al
Qaeda. Equally large majorities of Kerry supporters perceive
the Bush administration as expressing these views. Seventy-three
percent say that the Bush administration asserts that Iraq had
WMD and 74% that Iraq was providing substantial support to al Qaeda.
Also, according to Steven Kull, “Another reason that Bush
supporters may hold to these beliefs is that they have not accepted
the idea that it does not matter whether Iraq had WMD or supported
al Qaeda. Here too they are in agreement with Kerry supporters.” When
asked whether the US should have gone to war against Iraq if US
intelligence had concluded that Iraq was not developing WMD or
providing support to al Qaeda, 58% of Bush supporters said the
US should not have, and 61% believe that the President would not
have.
Kull continues: “To support the President and to accept
that he took the US to war based on mistaken assumptions likely
creates substantial cognitive dissonance, and leads Bush supporters
to suppress [, repress, and deny] awareness [italics mine]
of unsettling information about prewar Iraq.”
The proclivity of Bush supporters to utilize the triad of hysterical
defenses, suppression, repression, and denial, to ignore dissonant
information also extends to other realms, a highly significant
finding.
Despite an abundance of evidence including polls conducted in
38 countries, only 31% of Bush supporters are able to recognize
and acknowledge that the majority of people in the world oppose
the US having gone to war with Iraq. Forty-two percent mistakenly
believe that views are evenly divided, and 26% mistakenly believe
that the majority approves. Among Kerry supporters, 74% are
able to acknowledge that the world is opposed to the US war of
aggression.
Likewise, 57% of Bush supporters mistakenly believe that the majority
of people in other nations favor Bush’s reelection, 33% assumed
that views are equally divided, and only 9% believed that Kerry
was preferred. A recent survey of 35 major countries around
the world revealed that in 30, a majority favored Kerry while in
just 3 was Bush favored.
Bush supporters also harbor gross misperceptions of Bush’s
international policy positions. Substantial majorities incorrectly
assume that Bush supports multilateral approaches to international
issues, the Comprehensive Test Ban Treaty (69%). the treaty banning
land mines (72%), and the Kyoto treaty addressing the problem of
global warming (51%). After Bush denounced the International
Criminal Court in the recent debates of Presidential candidates,
the perception that he favored it dropped from 66%, but still a
majority of 53% continue to believe that he favors it. Seventy-four
percent incorrectly believe that he favors including labor and
environmental standards in trade agreements. In all these
instances, majorities of Bush supporters favor the positions they
impute to Bush while Kerry supporters are much more accurate in
their perceptions of his positions on these issues.
To turn to Steven Kull again, “The roots of the Bush supporters’ resistance to information [italics
mine] very likely lie in the traumatic experience of 9/11 and equally
in the near pitch-perfect leadership that President Bush showed
in its immediate wake. This appears to have created a powerful
bond between Bush and his supporters – and an idealized
image [italics mine] of the President that makes it difficult
for his supporters to imagine that he could have made incorrect
judgments before the war, that world public opinion could be critical
of his policies or that the President could hold foreign policy
positions that are at odds with his supporters.”
I find Kull’s explanation of Bush supporters’ resistance
to information discrepant with pre-existing beliefs to be
the joint result of the traumatic experience of 9/11 and the
quality of the President’s leadership in the wake of that
event myopic and unconvincing.
Kull seems to be positing that the propensity of Bush supporters to
tune out discrepant information is situationally determined. He
does not seem to entertain the possibility that the behavior
may not wholly be situationally determined but, instead, may
also be, in part, dispositionally determined as well, that is,
the behavior may be an interaction of state-determined and trait-determined components.
Since Kerry supporters were also in the same situation as Bush
supporters, they, too, experienced 9/11 and Bush’s “leadership,” and
they, too, should respond like Bush supporters if resistance
to information discrepant with pre-existing beliefs was primarily state-determined but
they clearly do not. Why is that so?
Unfortunately, Kull is silent on this point so we will have to
look elsewhere for a more veridical explanation of this highly
significant difference in the behavior of Bush and Kerry supporters.
In a superb meta-analysis of the data from 50 years of research
on the psychological motives, traits, and tendencies that underlie
differences between the political right and left involving 88 samples,
12 countries, and 22,818 cases, Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, and Sulloway
[see Jost, John T., Glaser, Jack. Kruglanski, Arie W., and Sulloway,
Frank J. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), May 2003, 339 – 375]
confirmed that several psychological variables predict political
conservatism.
The researchers state: “The core ideology of conservatism
stresses resistance to change and justification of inequality [read: the
rationalization of structural racism and the racist exclusion of
out-groups] and is motivated by needs that vary situationally and
dispositionally to manage uncertainty and threat” (p. 339).
The variables predictive of political conservatism in the order
of their predictive validity are as follows: Death anxiety
or mortality salience (weighted mean r = .50); system instability
or threat to the stability of the social system (.47); dogmatism
and intolerance of ambiguity (.34); openness to new experience
(-.32); uncertainty tolerance (-.27); personal needs for order,
structure, and closure (.26); integrative complexity (-.20); and
fear of threat and loss (.18).
Let us briefly examine each of these factors.
Death Anxiety or Mortality Salience
As the salience of one’s own mortality concerns increases
through traumatic events like 9/11, ideological defensiveness and
political conservatism also increases.
The most thorough research program to assess the effects of mortality
salience on social and political attitudes has been carried out
by Greenberg et al. who have demonstrated that mortality salience
leads people to defend culturally valued norms and practices more
fiercely and to distance themselves from, and to derogate out-group
members. Death anxiety has also been linked to system justifying
forms of stereotyping and a greater preference for stereotype-consistent
women and minority group members (“Toms, Coons, Bucks, and
Mulattoes” to borrow a phrase).
In addition, mortality salience has been shown to elicit greater
punitiveness and aggression toward those who violate cultural values.
Greenberg et al. have also demonstrated that mortality salience
led high authoritarians to denigrate those who were dissimilar
to them but did not have this effect on low authoritarians. And
in another study by Greenberg et al., mortality salience was found
to increase political intolerance among conservatives
but it increased political tolerance among liberals, perhaps
because tolerance is an important attribute of the Weltanschauung
or worldview of the latter but not the former group.
The significant difference between conservatives and liberals,
political intolerance versus political tolerance,
suggests that death anxiety or mortality salience is not simply
the resultant of situational factors alone but reveals dispositional
features as well.
System Instability or Threat to the Stability of the Social
System
A large body of archival research demonstrates that during times
of social crisis, people are more likely to turn to authoritarian
leaders and institutions for security, stability, and structure. For
example, during periods of severe economic threat, the depression
years of 1930 – 1939, research has shown that people were
more likely to join authoritarian churches and less likely to join
non-authoritarian churches. Similarly, in another study which
examined years of heavy unemployment in Seattle, Washington, 1961,
1964, 1969, and 1970, they found higher than usual conversion rates
there for an authoritarian church and lower than usual conversion
rates for a non-authoritarian church, while in relatively good
economic years in Seattle, 1962, 1965, and 1966, coincided with
lower than usual conversion rates to the authoritarian church and
higher than usual conversion rates for the non-authoritarian church.
In another study, historians were recruited to rate all of the
US presidential election years between 1788 and 1992 on the degree
to which the social, economic, and political circumstances of that
period were threatening to the American established order. The
results showed that during system-threatening times, presidential
candidates who were rated as high on power motivation, forcefulness,
and strength were elected by larger margins of victory than during
non-threatening times.
These studies provide fairly strong support for the hypothesis
that threats to the stability of the social system increase politically
conservative choices, decisions, and judgments.
Dogmatism and Intolerance for Ambiguity
A number of studies have found that dogmatism consistently correlates
with authoritarianism, political-economic conservatism, and the
holding of right-wing opinions. Further, political conservatives
have been found to be more dogmatic, mentally rigid, and closed-minded
than are either liberals or moderates.
Research on intolerance of ambiguity has shown that this trait
is associated with ethnocentrism, authoritarianism, and political
conservatism.
And intolerance of ambiguity and rigidity is clearly revealed
in a statement made by George W. Bush at an international conference
of world leaders in Genoa, Italy: “I know what I believe
and I believe what I believe is right.” And on another
occasion, Bush informed a British reporter: “Look,
my job isn’t to try to nuance…. My job is to tell people
what I think.”
Openness to New Experience
Additional research indicates that conservatives score lower on
measures of extraversion and are less likely to seek out interaction
with others, score lower than others on measures of general sensation
seeking, and are less likely than others to value broad-mindedness,
imagination, and having an exciting life.
In an interesting experiment, conservatives were also found to
be less likely than non-conservatives to volunteer for psychology
experiments that required openness to experience, experiments on
aesthetic interest, fantasy productions, and self-reports on sexual
behavior, but more likely to volunteer for experiments on decision
making and humor. In other words, conservatives evidenced
an unwillingness to engage in tasks that were person-oriented but
more likely to engage in object-oriented tasks.
This research provides consistent evidence that people who hold
politically conservative attitudes are generally less open to new
and stimulating experiences.
Uncertainty Tolerance
We next turn to a series of studies that investigate the hypothesis
that conservatives find ambiguity and uncertainty threatening.
In a study of artistic preferences of people who scored either
high or low on a scale for conservatism, conservatives were found
to exhibit a strong preference for simple rather than complex paintings
and a weaker preference for representational rather than abstract
paintings.
Similarly, conservatives were found to prefer simple poems to
complex ones; unambiguous over ambiguous literary texts; familiar
over unfamiliar music; and familiar over unfamiliar stimuli.
These studies all converge to reveal that conservatives are less
tolerant of ambiguity, less open to new experiences, and more avoidant
of uncertainty compared with moderates and liberals.
Need for Order, Structure, and Closure
A number of theories hypothesize that conservatives have a heightened
motivational need for order and structure. The research that
exists is consistent with this hypothesis.
In summarizing this research, Jost et al. comment: “This
evidence is consistent not only with research on dogmatism, intolerance
of ambiguity, and uncertainty avoidance but also with the notion
that in the realm of political attitudes, authoritarians long for
order and structure, advocating such diverse measures as firm parental
discipline, comprehensive drug testing, core educational curricula,
and quarantines for AIDS patients . . . “ (p. 358).
Integrative Complexity
There is also a large body of research that examines left wing
and right wing differences in cognitive complexity. Content
analytic techniques have been developed to measure integrative
complexity that refers to the extent of differentiation among multiple
perspectives or dimensions and the higher order integration and
synthesis of these differentiated components.
For example, in a series of studies by Tetlock and his collaborators
that focused on the thinking style of political elites, there was
clear evidence that conservative ideologues were, in general, less
integratively complex than their liberal or moderate counterparts. Similarly,
data from an analysis of US senatorial speeches in 1975 and 1976
indicated that politicians whose voting records were classified
as either liberal or moderate showed significantly more integrative
complexity than did politicians with conservative voting records. These
results were repeated or replicated almost exactly in a study of
US Supreme Court justices. In another study of members of
the British House of Commons, the results showed that the most
integratively complex politicians were moderate socialists who
scored significantly higher on complexity than extreme socialists,
moderate conservatives, and extreme conservatives.
Fear of Threat and Loss
Right wing authoritarians, according to Altemeyer, “. .
. are scared. They see the world as a dangerous place, as
society teeters on the brink of self-destruction from evil and
violence. This fear seems to instigate aggression
in them. Second, right wing authoritarians tend to be highly
self-righteous. They think themselves much more moral and
upstanding than others – a self-perception considerably aided
by self-deception, their religious training, and some very efficient
guilt evaporators (such as going to confession). This self-righteousness disinhibits their
aggressive impulses and releases them to act out their fear-induced
hostilities.”
In keeping with the idea that conservatives see the world as threatening,
Altemeyer reported a study that found a strong, positive correlation
between the perception of a dangerous world and high scores on
the Right Wing Authoritarian Scale with a sample of Canadian college
students. Other researchers replicated this finding with
several samples in South Africa and New Zealand where they also
noted a significant correlation between the perception of a dangerous
world and scores on a Social Dominance Scale. Authoritarians
in these studies generally saw the world as a dangerous place while
liberals did not share this pessimistic perception.
In an ingenuous research program on the dream lives of liberals
and conservatives in the US, the investigator found that Republicans
reported three times as many nightmares as did Democrats. This
finding suggests that fear, danger, threat, and aggression percolates
more prominently through the unconscious life of conservatives
than liberals.
To the extent that conservatives are especially sensitive and
attuned to threat or possibility of loss, one reason they attempt
to maintain the statue quo, it follows that they should also be
more highly motivated by negatively framed outcomes or potential
losses than by positively framed outcomes or potential gains.
Five days before the 1996 US presidential election, researchers
presented high and lower scorers on the Right Wing Authoritarian
Scale with persuasive arguments that stressed either the potential
rewards of voting (“a way to express and live in accordance
with positive values”) or the potential cost of not voting
(“not voting allows others to take away your right to express
your values”). They found that high authoritarians
were more influenced by threatening messages than by reward messages
but the pattern was just the reverse for low authoritarians who
were more influenced by reward than threat messages.
In general, research indicates that a “prevention” orientation
that focuses on potential threats and losses is associated with
conservative ideas.
Conclusion
This brief review of the literature on the determinants of a conservative
orientation strongly suggests that while situational factors may
influence the experience and expression of conservatism, they are
not the whole story.
Since we have already seen that character rigidity and motivational
threat are related to the holding of conservative attitudes and
values, system instability and other threatening circumstances
should also increase conservative tendencies in the population.
A kind of matching process takes place whereby people adopt ideological
beliefs that are most likely to satisfy their psychological needs
and motives and ideology is a quintessential example of psychodynamically
induced cognition and preferred coping mechanisms in that people
are highly motivated to perceive the world in ways that satisfy
their needs, values, and prior beliefs and commitments.
Non-elites like the working and middle class might adopt conservative
ideologies under some circumstances in order to reduce fear and
anxiety, cognitive dissonance, uncertainty, or instability while
the more advantaged elites might embrace conservatism for reasons
of self-interest or social dominance and maintenance of their privileged
position.
We are now in a position to understand what Kull and his associates
at the University of Maryland could not understand in the data
from their survey of Bush and Kerry supporters: Why Bush
supporters were extremely resistant to information discrepant
with their pre-existing beliefs. This is a preferred
coping mechanism, a psychological trait or orientation, the utilization
of hysterical defenses of denial, suppression, and repression,
to allay anxiety, fear, cognitive dissonance and other disruptive,
dysphoric emotions evidenced more often in conservatives than in
people who are liberal and who seem to be more solidly grounded
in reality.
Conservatism as a system of belief is a function of many different
kinds of factors. Politically conservative orientations are
multiply-determined by a wide variety of factors that vary personally
and situationally and have both a stable definitional core and
a set of more malleable, situationally determined, historically
changing peripheral associations.
It is the ideological core of political conservatism more than
its peripheral aspects that seems to be linked to specific social,
cognitive, and psychodynamic needs.
A distinguishing mark of political conservatism is fear of change
and the self-definitions of liberals and conservatives have to
do with acceptance of, versus resistance to, change.
Another core issue important to African Americans, other people
of color, and out-groups concerns endorsement or preference for
inequality. Liberals favor greater equality while conservatives
perceive society as naturally hierarchical and are more explicitly
racist.
Finally, there is a real culture war under way in the US between
the “Blues” who reside in the liberal, cultural and
intellectual metropolis, major cities, college towns,
etc. and the “Reds” who are found in the more provincial
outlying districts, two worlds which are separate, unequal,
and often, mutually unintelligible to each other.
Alvin Wyman Walker, PhD, PD is a Clinical Psychologist/Psychotherapist
who earned PhDs in Personality/Social Psychology and Clinical
Psychology. To hone his psychotherapeutic and psychoanalytic
skills, he spent six additional years of post-doctoral training
at a psychoanalytic institute.
Dr. Walker is currently in private practice in Harlem, New
York. |